Vita:Maslow-piramis

Az oldal más nyelven nem érhető el.
Új téma nyitása
A Wikipédiából, a szabad enciklopédiából
Legutóbb hozzászólt 89.134.199.32 5 évvel ezelőtt a(z) hjivatkozott forrás minősége témában
Ez a szócikk témája miatt a Pszichológiai műhely érdeklődési körébe tartozik.
Bátran kapcsolódj be a szerkesztésébe!
Bővítendő Ez a szócikk bővítendő besorolást kapott a kidolgozottsági skálán.
Nagyon fontos Ez a szócikk nagyon fontos besorolást kapott a műhely fontossági skáláján.
Értékelő szerkesztő: Misibacsi; Partmoso (vita), értékelés dátuma: 2017. március 27.
Pszichológiai témájú szócikkek Wikipédia:Cikkértékelési műhely/Index

dátum=2009-08-31 A szócikknek illik egy rövid meghatározással kezdődnie. Itt a szócikk közepéig nem is esik szó a cikk tárgyáról. – Hkoala 2008. október 31., 15:34 (CET)Válasz

Nekem az a gondom ezzel a szócikkel, hogy az üzleti nézőpontból mutatja be a Maslow-piramist, holott ez egy pszichológiai elmélet, amelyben eredetileg nem "fogyasztók" szerepelnek, hanem "személyek". Lásd pl. a szócikk angol változatát. Ott tökéletesen, részletesen ki van fejtve a Maslow-piramis pszichológiai szemszögből, és később egy alpontban tér ki az üzleti alkalmazására. – MegaBrutal vita 2010. január 8., 14:31 (CET)Válasz

Mi a q-istennek ángliusul?[szerkesztés]

Hogyhogy nincs senki, aki azt a rohadt rajzot hajlandó lenne MAGYAR nyelvű szöveggel ellátni? – Holdkóros vita 2017. március 27., 14:31 (CEST)Válasz

hjivatkozott forrás minősége[szerkesztés]

itt van ez: Tamás Katalin: Hol tévedett Maslow?

forrásként felsorolva. és valóban szép színes ábrával szolgál a maslow-piramishoz.

namost a maslow piramisnak alig van köze maslowhoz, de szemléletességének és megkapó egyszerűségének köszönhetően az egész világon elterjedt: ha Maslow, akkor piramis. de, tekintettel arra, hogy a piramis olyan leegyszerűsítést sugall, ami a maslow-kritika több pontjának okot szolgáltat, miközben mslow 1943-as publikációja ezek zömét (talán egyedül a nem kielégítően egzakt kutatási módszertan kivételével) kitárgyalja (pl: az éhségsztrájkolók, éhező tudósok, kollektivista társadalmak/kultúrák, önfeláldozók, mártírok, és más eltérések a leírt általános motiváció-hierarchiától).

Kérdezem:tényleg egy hatásvadász, és szemlátomást az eredeti maslowi szöveget nem ismerő, ugyanakkor saját ezoterikus szimbolikában újracsomagolt maslow-(egy-pont-egyes)-verzióját és pénzért kapható magánszolgáltatását reklámozó, "hol tévedett Maslow" url-című honlap a legmegfelelőbb forrás a maslowi elmélet hiteles ismertetésére?

itt az eredeti maslowi szöveg egy részlete ami több hiányos pontjára hívja fel a figyelmet az értékhiererchia elméletnek, mint az összes félművelt bulvárkritikus az utána eltelt fél évszázadban:

III. FURTHER CHARACTERISTICS OF THE BASIC NEEDS The degree of fixity of the hierarchy of basic needs. -- We have spoken so far as if this hierarchy were a fixed order but actually it is not nearly as rigid as we may have implied. It is true that most of the people with whom we have worked have seemed to have these basic needs in about the order that has been indicated. However, there have been a number of exceptions. (1) There are some people in whom, for instance, self-esteem seems to be more important than love. This most common reversal in the hierarchy is usually due to the development of the notion that the person who is most likely to be loved is a strong or powerful person, one who inspires respect or fear, and who is self confident or aggressive. Therefore such people who lack love and seek it, may try hard to put on a front of aggressive, confident behavior. But essentially they seek high self-esteem and its behavior expressions more as a means-to-an-end than for its own sake; they seek self-assertion for the sake of love rather than for self-esteem itself. (2) There are other, apparently innately creative people in whom the drive to creativeness seems to be more important than any other counter-determinant. Their creativeness might appear not as self-actualization released by basic satisfaction, but in spite of lack of basic satisfaction. (3) In certain people the level of aspiration may be permanently deadened or lowered. That is to say, the less pre-potent goals may simply be lost, and may disappear forever, so that the person who has experienced life at a very low level, i. e., chronic unemployment, may continue to be satisfied for the rest of his life if only he can get enough food. (4) The so-called 'psychopathic personality' is another example of permanent loss of the love needs. These are people who, according to the best data available (9), have been starved for love in the earliest months of their lives and have simply lost forever the desire and the ability to give and to receive affection (as animals lose sucking or pecking reflexes that are not exercised soon enough after birth).[p. 387] (5) Another cause of reversal of the hierarchy is that when a need has been satisfied for a long time, this need may be underevaluated. People who have never experienced chronic hunger are apt to underestimate its effects and to look upon food as a rather unimportant thing. If they are dominated by a higher need, this higher need will seem to be the most important of all. It then becomes possible, and indeed does actually happen, that they may, for the sake of this higher need, put themselves into the position of being deprived in a more basic need. We may expect that after a long-time deprivation of the more basic need there will be a tendency to reevaluate both needs so that the more pre-potent need will actually become consciously prepotent for the individual who may have given it up very lightly. Thus, a man who has given up his job rather than lose his self-respect, and who then starves for six months or so, may be willing to take his job back even at the price of losing his a self-respect. (6) Another partial explanation of apparent reversals is seen in the fact that we have been talking about the hierarchy of prepotency in terms of consciously felt wants or desires rather than of behavior. Looking at behavior itself may give us the wrong impression. What we have claimed is that the person will want the more basic of two needs when deprived in both. There is no necessary implication here that he will act upon his desires. Let us say again that there are many determinants of behavior other than the needs and desires. (7) Perhaps more important than all these exceptions are the ones that involve ideals, high social standards, high values and the like. With such values people become martyrs; they give up everything for the sake of a particular ideal, or value. These people may be understood, at least in part, by reference to one basic concept (or hypothesis) which may be called 'increased frustration-tolerance through early gratification'. People who have been satisfied in their basic needs throughout their lives, particularly in their earlier years, seem to develop exceptional power to withstand present or future thwarting of these needs simply because they have strong,[p. 388] healthy character structure as a result of basic satisfaction. They are the 'strong' people who can easily weather disagreement or opposition, who can swim against the stream of public opinion and who can stand up for the truth at great personal cost. It is just the ones who have loved and been well loved, and who have had many deep friendships who can hold out against hatred, rejection or persecution. I say all this in spite of the fact that there is a certain amount of sheer habituation which is also involved in any full discussion of frustration tolerance. For instance, it is likely that those persons who have been accustomed to relative starvation for a long time, are partially enabled thereby to withstand food deprivation. What sort of balance must be made between these two tendencies, of habituation on the one hand, and of past satisfaction breeding present frustration tolerance on the other hand, remains to be worked out by further research. Meanwhile we may assume that they are both operative, side by side, since they do not contradict each other, In respect to this phenomenon of increased frustration tolerance, it seems probable that the most important gratifications come in the first two years of life. That is to say, people who have been made secure and strong in the earliest years, tend to remain secure and strong thereafter in the face of whatever threatens. Degree of relative satisfaction. -- So far, our theoretical discussion may have given the impression that these five sets of needs are somehow in a step-wise, all-or-none relationships to each other. We have spoken in such terms as the following: "If one need is satisfied, then another emerges." This statement might give the false impression that a need must be satisfied 100 per cent before the next need emerges. In actual fact, most members of our society who are normal, are partially satisfied in all their basic needs and partially unsatisfied in all their basic needs at the same time. A more realistic description of the hierarchy would be in terms of decreasing percentages of satisfaction as we go up the hierarchy of prepotency, For instance, if I may assign arbitrary figures for the sake of illustration, it is as if the average citizen [p. 389] is satisfied perhaps 85 per cent in his physiological needs, 70 per cent in his safety needs, 50 per cent in his love needs, 40 per cent in his self-esteem needs, and 10 per cent in his self-actualization needs. As for the concept of emergence of a new need after satisfaction of the prepotent need, this emergence is not a sudden, saltatory phenomenon but rather a gradual emergence by slow degrees from nothingness. For instance, if prepotent need A is satisfied only 10 per cent: then need B may not be visible at all. However, as this need A becomes satisfied 25 per cent, need B may emerge 5 per cent, as need A becomes satisfied 75 per cent need B may emerge go per cent, and so on. Unconscious character of needs. -- These needs are neither necessarily conscious nor unconscious. On the whole, however, in the average person, they are more often unconscious rather than conscious. It is not necessary at this point to overhaul the tremendous mass of evidence which indicates the crucial importance of unconscious motivation. It would by now be expected, on a priori grounds alone, that unconscious motivations would on the whole be rather more important than the conscious motivations. What we have called the basic needs are very often largely unconscious although they may, with suitable techniques, and with sophisticated people become conscious. Cultural specificity and generality of needs. -- This classification of basic needs makes some attempt to take account of the relative unity behind the superficial differences in specific desires from one culture to another. Certainly in any particular culture an individual's conscious motivational content will usually be extremely different from the conscious motivational content of an individual in another society. However, it is the common experience of anthropologists that people, even in different societies, are much more alike than we would think from our first contact with them, and that as we know them better we seem to find more and more of this commonness, We then recognize the most startling differences to be superficial rather than basic, e. g., differences in style of hair-dress, clothes, tastes in food, etc. Our classification of basic [p. 390] needs is in part an attempt to account for this unity behind the apparent diversity from culture to culture. No claim is made that it is ultimate or universal for all cultures. The claim is made only that it is relatively more ultimate, more universal, more basic, than the superficial conscious desires from culture to culture, and makes a somewhat closer approach to common-human characteristics, Basic needs are more common-human than superficial desires or behaviors. Multiple motivations of behavior. -- These needs must be understood not to be exclusive or single determiners of certain kinds of behavior. An example may be found in any behavior that seems to be physiologically motivated, such as eating, or sexual play or the like. The clinical psychologists have long since found that any behavior may be a channel through which flow various determinants. Or to say it in another way, most behavior is multi-motivated. Within the sphere of motivational determinants any behavior tends to be determined by several or all of the basic needs simultaneously rather than by only one of them. The latter would be more an exception than the former. Eating may be partially for the sake of filling the stomach, and partially for the sake of comfort and amelioration of other needs. One may make love not only for pure sexual release, but also to convince one's self of one's masculinity, or to make a conquest, to feel powerful, or to win more basic affection. As an illustration, I may point out that it would be possible (theoretically if not practically) to analyze a single act of an individual and see in it the expression of his physiological needs, his safety needs, his love needs, his esteem needs and self-actualization. This contrasts sharply with the more naive brand of trait psychology in which one trait or one motive accounts for a certain kind of act, i. e., an aggressive act is traced solely to a trait of aggressiveness.

(A Theory of Human Motivation; A. H. Maslow (1943); Originally Published in Psychological Review, 50, 370-396.) (http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Maslow/motivation.htm) 89.134.199.32 (vita) 2018. november 1., 00:56 (CET).Válasz